RESEARCH ARTICLE


The Impact of Socioeconomic Status on the Neural Substrates Associated with Pleasure



Michael E Silverman*, 1, Peter Muennig2, Xun Liu3, Zohn Rosen4, Martin A Goldstein5
1 Department of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, New York USA
2 Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, New York, New York USA
3 Department of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, New York USA
4 Psychology Department, City University of New York, Brooklyn, New York USA
5 Department of Neurology, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, New York USA


Article Metrics

CrossRef Citations:
22
Total Statistics:

Full-Text HTML Views: 1166
Abstract HTML Views: 1106
PDF Downloads: 402
Total Views/Downloads: 2674
Unique Statistics:

Full-Text HTML Views: 566
Abstract HTML Views: 726
PDF Downloads: 277
Total Views/Downloads: 1569



Creative Commons License
© Silverman et al; Licensee Bentham Open

open-access license: This is an open access article licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/) which permits unrestricted, non-commercial use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the work is properly cited.

* Address correspondence to this author at the Mount Sinai Medical Center, One Gustave L. Levy Place, Department of Psychiatry, Box 1230, New York, New York, 10029, USA; Tel: (212) 659-8813; E-mail: michael.silverman@mssm.edu
This study was funded by Grant Number MO1-RR-00071 from the National Center for Research Resources.


Abstract

Low socio-economic status (SES) is associated with increased morbidity and premature mortality. Because tonic adversity relates to a diminished ability to experience pleasure, we hypothesized that subjects living in poverty would show diminished neural responsivity to positive stimuli in regions associated with positive experience and reward. Visual images were presented to twenty-two subjects in the context of a EPI-BOLD fMRI paradigm. Significant differences in neural responses between SES groups to poverty vs. neutral images were assessed, examining group, condition, and interaction effects. The data suggest that persons living in low-SES have neural experiences consistent with diminished interest in things generally enjoyed and point toward a possible explanation for the relationship between socioeconomic inequalities and mood disorders, such as depression, by SES.

Keywords: fMRI, depression, mood, stress, socio-economic status.



INTRODUCTION

The relationship between socio-economic status (SES) and health, the income gradient—the relative increase in mortality with lower income—accounts for a greater loss of health than any risk factor other than normal human aging [1]. While the 37 million Americans living below the poverty line are most affected, the income gradient extends throughout the entire population including those who have comfortable housing, access to health insurance, and who can readily afford a healthy diet [2]. Such findings have sparked a movement in health disparities research to look beyond material disadvantage and towards possible psychosocial factors.

While there is a considerable amount of descriptive evidence for how SES might “get under the skin,” [3] to date, no SES focused research has examined how SES-associated psychological stimuli or processes could be transduced into disrupted autonomic function. Theories regarding how SES might affect health through psychological pathways include stress-induced pathophysiology, negative emotional style, and mental illness [4]. One possibility is that diminished life satisfaction, such as that associated with living in poverty while repeatedly being exposed to more preferable conditions impacts psychosocial health [5,6]. In this scenario, individuals exposed to aversive environmental stimuli develop anxiety and apprehension thereby increasing the risk of mental illness such as depression [7]. Indeed, select data support this hypothesis. For example, in the child psychiatry literature a diverse range of adverse life circumstances, both acute and chronic, increase the risk for developing depression in adult life [8]. Similarly, neuroimaging research has revealed abnormalities in cortical regions and brain circuitry both implicated in stress and depression [4,8,9]. As a result one theoretical approach has been to explain the well-documented and frequent co-occurrence of chronic stress in the development and severity of a wide range of illness and disease [10,11].

The following study explores the impact of SES on brain function in relation to reward circuitry responses using blood oxygen dependent (BOLD) fMRI and examines the neural response to positive affective stimuli in persons living in a low SES position compared to those in higher SES. Our hypothesis came from human and animal research demonstrating a diminished ability to experience pleasure or positive motivation in response to repeated adversity [8]. Because the effect of stress on brain function can be demonstrated by means of appropriate stimuli designed to activate neural substrates [12], we theorized that those in low SES positions would demonstrate decreased activation in areas commonly associated with the processing of positive affective stimuli, including regions associated with pleasure and reward in comparison to higher SES subjects.

MATERIALS AND METHODOLOGY

Subjects

22 African American subjects were scanned over the course of a year. 7 were subsequently discarded due to head movement greater than 1 voxel (3-4mm), leaving 15 subjects (6 males, 9 females; mean age=24; range 19–29 years; 8 low SES and 7 high SES). Prior to participation, each subject provided written informed consent as commensurate with the Mount Sinai School of Medicine Institutional Review Board. All subjects were right-handed, native English speakers, without a history of self-reported psychiatric difficulty including depression, head trauma, neuropsychiatric complication, and illicit chemical or alcohol dependence.

Subjects were categorized into high and low SES categories based on both childhood and current SES. To qualify for inclusion, subjects had to have been SES-concordant in both childhood and adulthood. The SES classification algorithm is available by request.

Paradigm

The fMRI activation paradigm consisted of an emotional image probe, with semantic specificity, allowing for a complementary higher-level examination of the hypothesized circuitry. This paradigm employs stimuli whose emotional qualities are incidental relative to the explicit nature of a picture/scrambled-image determination behavioral task demand (2-alternative-forced-choice; 2AFC). Using this technique, the evocation of potentially confounding cognitive processes (e.g., semantic categorization) could be minimized.

While we only report data on the stimulus conditions of interest (e.g. positive v. neutral; data from the other conditions have been reported elsewhere [13,14]), the entire stimulus set consisted of positive, negative, neutral and poverty images counterbalanced across visual orientation presented against a black background. Positive, negative, and neutral images were selected from the International Affective Picture System [15], while poverty images were validated in a similar manner using 50 volunteer participants.

Behavioral responses were based on picture/scrambled-image judgment cues. Subjects were instructed to perform a 2AFC right index finger button-press immediately upon presentation of a picture and perform a right middle finger button press upon presentation of a scrambled image. Corresponding button presses were counterbalanced across subjects. Subjects were not pre-informed of the emotional nature of the stimulus images.

The task was presented in a block design. Presentation was counterbalanced to control for order and time effects. Each block was comprised of 10 novel stimuli of the same valence; there were 100 trials per condition, 400 total trials per study session. Blocks were counterbalanced to include 1, 2 or 3 scrambled stimulus images. Each stimulus appeared for 1.5 second, followed by a jittered inter-stimulus interval (ISI) averaging 1900 msec., for a total block duration of 34 seconds. Each block was followed by 12 seconds of rest. Each run was preceded and followed by an additional 36-second rest period. During rest periods, subjects were instructed to look at a centered fixation cross with their minds blank or floating freely. Stimulus presentation and response collection were performed within the E-Prime environment (http://www.pstnet.com).

Immediately after exiting the scanner subjects were presented with a series of images via computer consisting of those seen during scanning (targets) randomly interspersed with an equal number of new images (distractors). Subjects were asked to indicate which images they believed were presented during the scanning session using a 2AFC (yes/no) button press. Because subjects were not informed of this task prior to entering the MRI, the purpose of this assessment was to explore any differences in the incidental encoding of the presented images. Following the completion of this task, subjects were asked to rate a similarly counter-balanced subset of target images presented on a touch screen monitor along a Likert-like scale according to emotional valence [15] in order to confirm stimulus categorizations.

Image Acquisition

Image data were acquired with a research-dedicated Siemens Allegra Magnetron 3 Tesla head dedicated MRI scanner (maximum gradient strength ~60mT/m, max gradient slew ~600T/m/s). T1-weighted spoiled gradient (MP-RAGE) MRI whole brain anatomical scans (208 slices; .82 mm in-plane resolution, .80 mm slice thickness, contiguous slices) were acquired followed by T2-weighted Turbo Spin Echo axial whole brain images (3mm slice thickness) to explore potential pathology. Finally, gradient echo planar imaging - BOLD fMRI were acquired (TR=2000msec, TE=30, 32 slices; 3mm thickness; Imm gap) as an index of neuronal activity during the 2AFC activation paradigm.

Imaging Processing and Data Analysis

Prior to statistical analysis, the first two volumes of each run were discarded to allow the MR signal to reach steady state. The remaining images in each participant’s time series were motion corrected using the MCFLIRT module of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library, v3.3) package (http://www.-fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Images in the data series were spatially smoothed with a 3-D Gaussian kernel (FWHM = 8mm), and temporally filtered using a high-pass filter (320 seconds). The FEAT (FMRIB’s Expert Analysis Tool) module of FSL package was used for these steps and later statistical analysis.

Customized square waveforms were generated for each individual with onset timing (on/off) generated according to the order of experimental conditions. These waveforms are convolved with a double gamma hemodynamic response function. For each participant, FILM (FMRIB’s Improved Linear Model) was used with local autocorrelation correction to estimate the hemodynamic parameters for four explanatory variables (EVs; neutral, positive, negative, and poverty) and to generate statistical contrast maps of interest. The six movement parameters (i.e., translation and rotation of x, y, and z axes) were then modeled as covariates.

The five experimental runs for each participant were analyzed separately and the average of these five runs for each individual obtained through a higher-level analysis using the FLAME (FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects) module (stage 1 only). Contrast maps were warped into common stereotaxic space before mixed-effects group analyses were performed. The normalization procedure involved registering the average EPI image to the MP-RAGE (T1) image from the same participant, and then to the ICBM152 T1 template, using the FLIRT (FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool) module.

Statistical maps were thresholded at Z>2.81, p<0.005 two-tailed uncorrected with local peaks (maximal activation) located using the Mintun peak algorithm [16]. Small volume corrections (p≤0.05) were then applied to a priori anatomically based regions of interest including the anterior and posterior cingulate, insula and ventral striatum.

RESULTS

Demographic Data

The demographic characteristics of subjects are reported in Table 1. Of note, low SES subjects were more likely to have a lower educational attainment (p ≤ 0.006), lower household income (p ≤ 0.031), and lower scores on the COMQOL productivity measure (p ≤ 0.021) than high SES subjects.

Table 1.

Mean, Standard Deviation and Significance for Demographic Characteristics of the fMRI Included Sub-Sample by SES Group


Demographic Measure Low SES (N=8) High SES (N=7) Significance
Gender 4 Male
4 Female
2 Male
5 Female
n/a
Age 22.63 (2.77) 25.57 (2.51) t=2.15, p=.051
Education 2.62 (1.06) 4.14 (0.69) Z=2.75,p=.006**
Current Household Income $29,956 ($14,543) $56,714 ($24,464) t=2.53, p=.031*
Num. of People who Sleep in Home 3.13 (1.55) 2.14 (1.35) t=1.30, p=.216
Perceived Stress Scale 17.38 (5.78) 16.14 (5.27) t=0.43, p=.675
Perceived Neighborhood Stress 30.50 (9.18) 33.00 (6.14) t=0.61, p=.553
Rosenberg Self Esteem 17.75 (8.48) 16.00 (4.32) t=0.49, p=.631
Comprehensive Quality of Life Inventory
Material Well-Being 8.63 (1.60) 10.00 (0.82) t=2.05, p=.061
Health 13.38 (1.51) 14.29 (0.49) t=1.53, p=.151
Productivity 13.00 (1.85) 15.43 (1.72) t=2.62, p=.021*
Intimacy 12.38 (2.26) 13.86 (0.90) t=1.62, p=.130
Safety 11.50 (2.20) 11.43 (1.27) t=0.08, p=.941
Emotional Well-Being 9.00 (2.07) 10.86 (1.35) t=2.02, p=.064
MacArthur Psychosocial Ladder 4.88 (2.23) 6.00 (2.04) t=1.01, p=.330
Social Support 0.87 (0.64) 1.00 (0.58) t=0.39, p=.700
Negative Life Events 2.14 (0.38) 1.79 (0.39) t=1.73, p=.109

Note. Data are mean (SD) except where indicated.

p<.05

**  p <.01

†  Education was measured on a 5-point scale with a higher value indicating a greater level of education (<H.S., H.S. Diploma, Some College, College Degree, Graduate/Professional Education). Analysis conducted using a Mann–Whitney U.

Behavioral Performance

Reaction times (RT) to the 2AFC task demonstrated a significant effect of stimulus condition (positive, neutral, negative and poverty trials: F(3,15) = 5.137, p ≤ 0.012). Further analyses of stimulus conditions relevant to the current hypothesis by group (low SES vs high SES) revealed a significant RT difference during positive image stimulus conditions (F(1,14) = 5.999, p ≤ 0.028) with faster responses being made by the high SES group (mean RT = 586.62ms) compared to the low SES group (mean RT = 730.08ms). No RT differences were observed between groups during neutral image stimulus conditions (F(1,14) = 3.602, n.s).

Post-Scan Behavioral Performance

Recognition Accuracy

Signal detection analysis (d’) for the incidental recognition of the two stimulus image conditions relevant to the hypothesis (positive and neutral) revealed no incidental recognition differences between high and low SES groups (positive stimuli, t(16)=1.49, n.s.; neutral stimuli, t(16)= 1.75, n.s.). However, analysis of the overall recognition accuracy differences between condition revealed a significant difference between neutral and positive image recognition (t(17)=2.156, p=0.046). Post-hoc within group analysis revealed that even though subjects were not informed of a post-scan recognition assessment prior to scanning, differences were observed in the recognition accuracy with neutral images being recalled better than positive images by the high SES group (high SES t(8)=2.718, p=.026; low SES t(8)=0.898, n.s).

Valence Rating

Analysis of the post-scan ratings of all stimulus images (positive, negative, poverty, and neutral) confirmed our assignment of stimuli to positive, negative, poverty and or neutral categories (F(3,60)=80.73, p≤.001) respectively. It is therefore fair to assume that the images employed in this study were reasonable probes of emotional processing within the participating subject population.

fMRI Results

Contrasts defined as low SES > high SES, positive vs. neutral revealed significantly less activation to positive stimuli in low SES subjects relative to higher SES subjects. Neuronal responses significant at p<.05, corrected for multiple comparisons were observed in the insula (right: [42,8,-10], Z=-3.66), fusiform (left:[-18,-2,-38], Z=-3.16), anterior cingulate (subgenual: [0,38,2], Z=-3.13;), posterior cingulate (left [-2,-34,14], Z=-3.76), caudate (left: [-2,16,10], Z=-3.13); right: [12,-2,28], Z=-3.07), pons (left: [-6,-14,-22], Z=-3.79), hippocampus (right: [34,-38,6], Z=-2.91). Posthoc analysis confirmed that there was no baseline shift that affected the group differences on the positive versus neutral contrast. In other words, the results observed between the two contrasts (neutral v positive) did not arise from low SES subjects having increased activations nor high SES subjects having attenuated activations to the ‘neutral’ stimuli.

DISCUSSION

Emerging evidence suggests that psychological stress may result in behavioral and autonomic changes placing one at greater risk of disease [4]. That a behavioral performance difference as measured by reaction time for positive images but not for neutral images in a scrambled/unscrambled judgment task lends further support to the assumption that even basic motor processes can become variable across simple choice tasks in the context of emotional tone in those living in lower SES. In such light, it is interesting that mood congruent biases in cognition and emotional responsitivity are among the most commonly reported research findings in the neuropsychiatric literature on depression – the disorder most commonly examined in association with low SES [17].

Notably, while the phenomenology of depression consists of an accentuation of negative affective processing it also consists of an inability to experience pleasure or positive motivation. That the subgenual anterior cingulate (sgACC) and posterior cingulate both demonstrated reduced responsivity to positive images in those in low-SES compared to those in higher SES (Fig. 1A) is consistent with recent literature demonstrating volumetric deficits in the sgACC [9] and abnormally decreased blood flow in other areas associated with reward and pleasure in depression [18].

Fig. (1).

low SES > high SES, positive vs. neutral. Threshold at p=.05 for visualization. Regions marked in blue (superimposed on an anatomical image) had statistically significant BOLD decreases whereas regions in red indicate significant BOLD increases.

A) Cingulate (anterior and posterior); B) Caudate (yellow), Insula (red); C) Pons; D) Insula.


The relative down regulation of the caudate in those in low-SES compared to higher SES (Fig. 1B) is particularly interesting in light of this region's prominent role in the processing of pleasure, reward and anticipated satisfaction [19,20]. Indeed, cerebral blood flow and metabolism have been shown to be abnormally decreased in the caudate in subjects diagnosed with major depression and bipolar disorder [21]. Consistent with this, a recent fMRI study found that depressed patients demonstrated significantly less ventral striatum activation to positive linguistic stimuli [18]. That the neural activation in the caudate, an area implicated in motivation and action [22], differed strongly between SES groups offers additional evidence to implicate economic stress in the development of depression and possibly other mood disturbances via processes occurring in the autonomic nervous system (e.g. pons [23]; Fig. 1C) and cortical regions associated with mediating reciprocal modulation of cognitive, heteromodal sensory, limbic, and autonomic control [24]. Interestingly, the insula which is histologically, topographically, and connectively poised to mediate reciprocal modulation of polymodal sensory, cognitive, emotional, and cerebrogenic autonomic control demonstrated both increased and decreased regulation during the activation paradigm (Fig. 1B & D). Notably, increased regulation in the anterior insula (especially left, Figure 1;B) has been demonstrated during anticipation of a unpleasant stimuli [25], while lowered activity (at the functional level) has been associated with long term chronic stress (e.g. posttraumatic stress disorder; PTSD) [26]. In such light, the conceptualization of low-SES conditions acting as a chronic stressor similar to other extended stress related disorders such as PTSD requires further exploration.

Finally, considerable evidence points to maladaptive emotional, behavioral and cognitive responses to stressful life circumstance as a hallmark of mood disorders and while those living in lower-SES are reported to be at higher risk for mood disorders [27], questions remain concerning the causal direction. Two suspected routes are social selection, which posits that individuals with psychopathology have a reduced ability to hold jobs and therefore a lower SES (referred to as social drift), and social causation, which posits that people with low SES develop psychiatric disorders as a result of living with adversity. Evidence for psychiatric social selection has been demonstrated by a handful of studies in which early-onset psychiatric disorders were shown to lead to reduced educational attainment [28], that severe early-onset disorders like schizophrenia were shown to lead to reduced income in adulthood [29], and that attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder lead to lower adult SES [30]. A concern with this literature base, however, is that many studies relating SES to psychopathology have involved child/adolescent samples, making the likelihood of social selection in these cases low as subjects tend to be too young to account for a significant portion of their families’ income (with the exception of an indirect effect, in which the child’s issues reduces the parents ability to maintain their income). Rather, an overwhelming majority of results from longitudinal studies provides more evidence in support of the social causation hypothesis [2,31]. Certainly, in most cases, a combined effect of both factors is likely responsible - as psychopathology can manifest in a multitude of ways and with a variety of time courses.

CONCLUSION

The intent of this study was to determine whether individuals living in low SES would respond differently than individuals living in higher SES to positively valenced stimuli in the context of a behavioral EPI-BOLD fMRI activation paradigm. We hypothesized that such differences would be tied to cortical regions associated with positive affect, reward and motivation. The importance of the current findings lie in the support they lend to understanding the specific nature of emotion processing and behavioral abnormalities among low-SES individuals and prove helpful in laying the groundwork for determining the mechanisms responsible for health disparities across the social gradient. While the data indicate that living in low-SES is associated with biases specific to positively valenced stimuli, we are now left with a number of questions. Namely, does the observed dysfunction arise from or give rise to depressive states? If the latter, than what accounts for the higher depression rates and health risks associated with lower SES and what role, if any, do they have in contributing to the maintenance of the social economic position?

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank Pamela Burke, Frank Macaluso and Hanna Oltarzewska for their technical assistance and Dr. Yasmin Hurd for reviewing this manuscript prior to submission.

REFERENCES

[1] Muennig P, Franks P, Jia H, Lubetkin E, Gold MR. The income-associated burden of disease in the United States Soc Sci Med 2005; 61(9): 2018-6.
[2] Costello JE, Compton SN, Keeler G, Angold A. Relationships between poverty and psychopathology, a natural experiment J Am Med Assoc 2003; 290: 2023-9.
[3] McEwen BS, Mirsky AE. How socioeconomic status may "get under the skin" and affect the heart Eur Heart J 2002; 23(22): 1727-8.
[4] McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators N Engl J Med 1998; 338(3): 171-9.
[5] Piko BF, Fitzpatrick KM. Socioeconomic status, psychosocial health and health behaviours among Hungarian adolescents Eur J Public Health 2007; 17(4): 353-60.
[6] Cattell V. Poor people, poor places, and poor health: the mediating role of social networks and social capital Soc Sci Med 2001; 52(10): 1501-6.
[7] Wilkinson RG. Health, hierarchy, and social anxiety Ann NY Acad Sci 1999; 896: 48-63.
[8] Kendler K, Karkowsky L, Prescott C. Causal relationship between stressful life events and onset of major depression Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156: 837-41.
[9] Drevets WC, Savitz J, Trimble M. The subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in mood disorders CNS Spectr 2008; 13(8): 663-81.
[10] Brown G, Harris T, Eales M. Social factors and comorbidity of depressive and anxiety disorders Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168: 50-7.
[11] Bauer ME, Jeckel CM, Luz C. The role of stress factors during aging of the immune system Ann NY Acad Sci 2009; 1153: 139-52.
[12] Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Fera F, Weinberger DR. Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli Biol Psychiatry 2003; 53(6): 494-501.
[13] Silverman M, Muennig P, Stitzer M, Rosen Z, Naidich T, Goldstein M. Enhanced attentional and viscero-integrative network activation in response to poverty-related image stimuli by low socio-economic status subjects: an emotional visual fMRI study In: Poster presented at the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society; April 14; San Francisco, CA. 2008.
[14] Guilliam D, Silverman M, Rosen Z, Muennig P Goldstein M. Functional neuroanatomic differences between low and high socio-economic status: An emotional visual fMRI study In: Poster presented at the 60th Annual Academy of Neurology (AAN) Conference; April 16; Chicago, IL. 2008.
[15] Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): technical manual and affective ratings The Center for Research in Psychophysiology Gainesville FL: University of Florida 1999.
[16] Mintun MA, Fox PT, Raichle ME. A highly accurate method of localizing regions of neuronal activation in the human brain with positron emission tomography J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1989; 9: 96-103.
[17] Gilman SE, Kawachi I, Fitzmaurice GM, Buka SL. Socioeconomic status in childhood and the lifetime risk or major depression Int J Epidemiol 2002; 31: 359-67.
[18] Epstein J, Pan H, Kocsis J, et al. Lack of ventral striatal response to positive stimuli in depressed versus normal subjects Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163: 1784-90.
[19] Schultz W. Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms Curr Opin Neurobiol 1997; 7: 191-7.
[20] de Quervain D, Fischbacher U, Treyer V, et al. The neural basis of altruistic punishment Science 2004; 305: 1254-8.
[21] Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 2002; 16: 331-48.
[22] Zald DH, Boileau I, El-Dearedy W, et al. Dopamine transmission in the human striatum during monetary reward tasks J Neurosci 2004; 24: 4105-12.
[23] Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Josephs O, et al. Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence Brain 2003; 126: 2139-52.
[24] Nagai M, Kishi K, Kato S. Insular cortex and neuropsychiatric disorders: a review of recent literature Eur Psychiatry 2007; 22: 387-94.
[25] Herwig U, Abler B, Walter H, Erk S. Expecting unpleasant stimuli - An fMRI study Psychiatry Res 2007; 154: 1-12.
[26] Simmons A, Strigo IA, Matthews SC, et al. Initial evidence of a failure to activate right anterior insula during affective set shifting in posttraumatic stress disorder Psychosom Med 2009; 71: 373-7.
[27] Kessler RC, Price RH, Wortman CB. Social factors in psychopathology: stress, social support, and coping processes Ann Rev Psychol 1991; 36: 531-72.
[28] Kessler RC, Foster CL, Saunders WB, Stang PE. Social consequences of psychiatric disorders, I: educational attainment Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152: 1026-32.
[29] Goldberg EM, Morrison SL. Schizophrenia and social class Br J Psychiatry 1963; 109: 785-802.
[30] Miech RA, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Wright BRE, Silvia PA. Low socioeconomic status and mental disorders: A longitudinal study of selection and causation during young adulthood Am J Sociol 1999; 104: 1096-31.
[31] Shaw DW, Winslow EB, Owens EB, Hood N. Young children's adjustment to chronic family adversity: a longitudinal study of low-income families J Am Acad Child Adolese Psychiatry 1998; 37: 545-3.