All published articles of this journal are available on ScienceDirect.
Neural Substrates Underlying Learning-Related Changes of the Unconditioned Fear Response
Abstract
The ability to predict an impending threat during Pavlovian conditioning diminishes the emotional response that is produced once the threat is encountered. Diminution of the threat response appears to be mediated by somewhat independent associative learning and expectancy-related processes. Therefore, the present study was designed to better understand the neural mechanisms that support associative learning processes, independent of expectancy, that influence the emotional response to a threat. Healthy volunteers took part in a Pavlovian conditioning procedure during which trait anxiety, expectation of the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), skin conductance response (SCR), and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) signal were assessed. The results showed no evidence for associative learning that was independent of expectation. Threat-related SCR expression was diminished on predictable trials vs. unpredictable trials of the UCS (i.e. conditioned UCR diminution). Similar to SCR, conditioned UCR diminution was observed within the left dorsolateral PFC, dorsomedial PFC, ventromedial PFC, and left anterior insula. In contrast, potentiation of the threat-related fMRI signal response was observed within left dorsolateral PFC, inferior parietal lobule (IPL), and posterior insula. A negative relationship was observed between UCS expectancy and UCR expression within the dorsomedial PFC, ventromedial PFC, and anterior insula. Finally, the anticipatory fMRI signal responses within the PFC, posterior cingulate, and amygdala showed an inverse relationship with threat-related activation within the brain regions that showed UCR diminution. The current findings suggest that the PFC and amygdala support learning-related processes that impact the magnitude of the emotional response to a threat.
INTRODUCTION
Fear is considered an important defense mechanism due to its evolutionary role in survival [1-3]. The ability to form associations between a dangerous event and the cues that predict it allows an organism to better adapt to a changing environment [1, 4]. An important aspect of this type of associative learning (i.e. Pavlovian fear conditioning) is that it allows an organism to more effectively avoid, escape, or minimize the impact of an impending threat [3-7]. Thus, from a functional perspective it is the response to the threat itself (i.e. unconditioned stimulus: UCS) that directly impacts survival and therefore may be the most biologically relevant feature of fear learning [4].
During Pavlovian fear conditioning, a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS) is paired with an aversive UCS. The conditioned response (CR) produced by the CS is typically used to index fear expression. Traditionally, CR expression is taken as evidence that an association between the CS and UCS has been formed. In contrast, the unconditioned response (UCR) is generally considered an automatic, unlearned reaction to the aversive UCS. However, prior work has shown that unconditioned skin conductance responses (SCRs) diminish as associations are formed during conditioning [8–12]. For example, UCR amplitude is decreased to paired vs. unpaired presentations of the CS and UCS [9, 13, 14]. Further, conscious expectation of the UCS has also been shown to diminish UCR expression [10, 15-17]. For example, UCR amplitude shows a greater decrease when participants expect a UCS compared to when the UCS is unexpected [10, 15]. This effect is generally referred to as conditioned UCR diminution.
Prior neuroimaging studies have employed Pavlovian fear conditioning to investigate the neural correlates of conditioned UCR diminution [10, 15, 17, 18]. These studies have demonstrated a learning-related decrease in UCR expression within multiple regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) [e.g. dorsolateral (dlPFC), dorsomedial (dmPFC), ventromedial (vmPFC)], cingulate cortex [e.g. anterior (ACC), posterior (PCC)], inferior parietal lobule (IPL), anterior insula, and amygdala [10, 15, 17, 18]. The findings from this prior work are consistent with other research that suggests the PFC regulates the emotional response [19, 20]. For example, prior work indicates that the PFC projects to the amygdala and provides regulatory control over emotion-related processes during fear conditioning [1, 19, 21]. Further, the threat-related fMRI signal response within the PFC and amygdala impacts the amplitude of the autonomic response (e.g. SCR) that is produced [10, 15, 17, 22]. This process appears to be critical for normal, healthy emotional function.
Converging lines of research indicate that healthy emotion regulation relies upon the PFC [19, 20, 23-25], and that anxiety disorders may be linked to insufficient regulatory control from the PFC. Further, PFC dysregulation is associated with increased amygdala reactivity [26-31] and an exaggerated emotional response to threats [11, 32, 33]. For example, prior work has shown that participants with low trait anxiety exhibit greater vmPFC activation compared to participants with high trait anxiety during cued fear conditioning [34]. In contrast, individuals with high trait anxiety showed a diminished vmPFC response that was associated with greater fear conditioned SCRs compared to participants with low trait anxiety [34]. Further, our prior work has demonstrated that unconditioned fMRI signal responses from several brain regions fluctuate with trait anxiety level [17]. Specifically, trait anxiety varied with dlPFC, dmPFC, PCC, and IPL activity such that, as trait anxiety level increased the threat-related fMRI signal response within these brain areas increased [17]. These studies suggest that anxiety level influences the magnitude of anticipatory and threat-related brain activation, which in turn influences the peripheral expression of emotion.
Associative learning and expectancy processes are additional factors that modulate the response produced by a threat. Prior work has demonstrated a reduction in the magnitude of the fMRI signal response once a cue-outcome relationship is established and predictable [35, 36]. However, when an outcome violates expectations an increase in the magnitude of brain activity is observed [35, 36]. Further, the magnitude of brain activation in response to an aversive stimulus is dependent upon the expectation of whether the aversive outcome is a certainty or only a possibility [15, 37, 38]. Prior work has demonstrated that the amplitude of the threat-elicited fMRI signal response within regions of the PFC, insula, cingulate, IPL, and amygdala varies with UCS expectancy [10, 15, 38]. More specifically, as UCS expectancy increases during the CS presentation, the amplitude of the threat response decreases. These findings suggest that conditioned UCR diminution is in part mediated by expectation of the UCS that is supported by regions of the PFC. In turn, these conditioned changes in the brain’s response to a threat appear to modify the peripheral emotional response that is expressed. For example, several studies have shown that as UCS expectancy increases the magnitude of unconditioned SCR expression decreases [10, 11, 15]. Further, these findings parallel the conditioned changes observed within the threat-related fMRI signal response [10, 15, 17, 38]. Taken together, these findings suggest that conscious expectation of an imminent threat may play an important role in modulation of the emotional response produced. However, prior work has also demonstrated threat-elicited SCRs that did not diminish as expectation increased [17]. For example, unconditioned SCRs produced by a UCS that followed a CS− did not differ from SCRs to a UCS presented alone even though UCS expectancies differed between these conditions [17]. Prior work has also demonstrated greater UCR diminution to a UCS that followed a CS+ compared to a UCS presented after a CS− even when UCS expectancy ratings were equivalent [11]. In addition, diminished unconditioned SCRs have been observed to a UCS that followed a CS+ compared to a UCS that followed a CS− even after participants were informed that the UCS would follow both the CS+ and CS− [12]. Together, these studies suggest that modulation of the threat-related emotional response is not solely mediated by conscious expectations. Instead, the findings suggest that associative learning processes independent of UCS expectations also influence UCR expression.
Given that prior work suggests UCR diminution is in part mediated by expectancy-independent processes, the present study was designed to better understand associative learning processes that influence UCR expression in the absence of differential UCS expectancies. The aim of this study was to determine the neural substrates that support expectancy-independent conditioned diminution of the emotional response to a threat. Based on prior work, we expected to observe a learning-related decrease in the fMRI signal response produced by the UCS independent of UCS expectancy [11]. Given the importance of the amygdala, dlPFC, dmPFC, and vmPFC in emotion [1, 19, 20, 38], we hypothesized that these brain regions would show UCR diminution during Pavlovian conditioning. Further, we expected threat-related brain activation within these brain regions to vary with individual differences in trait anxiety level. In turn, we expected threat-related amygdala activity to vary with the amplitude of unconditioned SCR expression [17, 22, 39-41].
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants:
Twenty-one healthy right-handed volunteers participated in this study [8 male, 13 female; age = 23.05 ± 0.82 years (mean ± SEM); range = 19-34 years]. All participants were included in the UCS expectancy and fMRI data analyses. However, four non-responsive (SCR < 0.05 μSiemens) participants were excluded from SCR data analyses. Thus, a total of seventeen participants were included in the SCR analyses (7 male, 10 female; age = 23.59 ± 0.95 years; range = 19-34 years). Fifteen participants were included in the secondary fMRI data analyses that examined the relationship between behavior and brain activation (6 male, 9 female; age = 23.87 ± 1.06 years; range = 19-34 years). The six participants excluded from these secondary fMRI analyses consisted of the four participants with non-responsive SCR and two participants that did not complete the trait anxiety assessment. All participants provided written informed consent.
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory:
Prior to the conditioning session, volunteers completed the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Form Y) for Adults [42]. The STAI is a self-assessment questionnaire that measures state and trait anxiety in terms of general negative affect [43]. The state scale reflects anxiety level at the current moment, whereas the trait scale reflects anxiety level experienced in general [42].
Stimuli:
The conditioned and unconditioned stimuli were presented through MR-compatible pneumatic headphones. Two tones (1025 and 1050 Hz; 10 s duration; 20 s ITI) that were difficult to discriminate served as the CSs. Our pilot work indicated that stimuli presented at these frequencies can be differentiated when presented back-to-back, but are difficult to discriminate when separated by a 20 s ITI. A loud white-noise served as the UCS (100 db, 0.5 s duration). Stimuli were presented in a pseudorandom order with the restriction that no more than two trials of the same stimulus could be presented consecutively.
Procedure:
Volunteers were exposed to a differential fear conditioning procedure in which the UCS coterminated with one tone (CS+) and the second tone was presented alone (CS−) during the acquisition phase. The CSs were counterbalanced across participants. The acquisition phase consisted of four 590 s scans. A total of 32 trials of each CS were presented during the acquisition phase (8 trials of each CS were presented in each scan). In addition, each acquisition scan included 3 test trials that consisted of UCS presentations that coterminated with the CS+ (CS+UCS) and CS− (CS−UCS), as well as presentations of the UCS alone (Fig. 1). Additional details on trial order during the acquisition phase have been published previously [17]. The acquisition phase was followed by a 920 s test phase that consisted of 30 test trials (10 CS+UCS trials, 10 CS−UCS trials, 10 UCS alone trials). In total, there were 14 test trials for each stimulus (4 from the acquisition phase, 10 from the test phase). The 14 test trials were grouped into the first seven test trials (Early test trials) and the last seven test trials (Late test trials) for further analysis. The test trials were binned in this manner to evaluate learning-related changes in UCS expectancy in a manner consistent with our prior work using CS presentations that were easy to discriminate [17].
UCS expectancy:
UCS expectancy was collected throughout the conditioning session. This measure was used to assess participants’ expectation of the UCS and determine whether the relationship between the CS and UCS had been learned. Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.; Albany, CA), was used to display a UCS expectancy rating scale on an IFIS-SA LCD (Invivo Corp.; Gainesville, FL) video screen. The video screen was located above the participant’s head and viewed through a mirror attached to the RF coil. Participant’s used an MRI compatible joystick (Current Designs; Philidelphia, PA) to control the rating bar on the video screen. Subjects were instructed to rate their UCS expectancy on a moment-by-moment basis using a continuous scale from 0 to 100 (0 = certain the UCS would not be presented, 50 = uncertain whether the UCS would be presented, 100 = certain the UCS would be presented) to reflect their current UCS expectancy. UCS expectancy was calculated as the average response (1 s sample) at UCS onset. Additional details on this methodology have been previously published [44].
Skin conductance response:
An MRI compatible physiological monitoring system (Biopac Systems; Goleta, CA) was used to collect SCR data as described in prior work [44]. SCR was sampled (2,000 Hz) with a pair of disposable radio-translucent electrodes (1 cm diameter, Biopac Systems; Goleta, CA) from the distal phalanx of the middle and ring fingers of the nondominant hand. SCR data were processed using Biopac AcqKnowledge 4.1 software. A 1 Hz low pass digital filter was applied and SCR data were resampled at 250 Hz. Unconditioned SCRs were limited to those that occurred within 10 s following the UCS presentation. Unconditioned SCRs smaller than 0.05 μSiemens were scored as 0.
Functional MRI:
Neuroimaging was completed on a Siemens 3 Tesla Allegra MRI system. High-resolution anatomical images (MPRAGE) were acquired in the sagittal plane using a T1 weighted series (TR=2300 ms, TE=3.9 ms, flip angle=12°, FOV=25.6 cm, matrix=256 x 256, slice thickness=1 mm, 0.5 mm gap). Whole brain blood oxygen level dependent [45] fMRI was completed using a gradient-echo echoplanar pulse sequence [46] in an oblique-axial orientation (TR=2000 ms, TE=30 ms, flip angle=70º, FOV=24 cm, matrix=64 x 64, slice thickness=4 mm, no gap) during stimulus presentations. The Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software package [47] was used for the fMRI analyses. Echo-planar time series data were slice time corrected, motion corrected, concatenated, reregistered to the fifth volume of the first imaging block, and spatially blurred using a 4 mm full-width-at-half-maximum Gaussian filter.
Functional MRI data were analyzed using a deconvolution analysis that was conducted at the individual subject level. This multiple linear regression modeled the input from all stimuli using a gamma variate hemodynamic response function. Reference waveforms modeled the CS+, CS−, and UCS during the acquisition phase, the CS+ and CS− on test trials, joystick movement, and head motion parameters. The regressors of interest modeled brain activation elicited by the UCS during the test trials (i.e. CS+UCS, CS−UCS, and the UCS alone). In this analysis, separate reference waveforms were used for the Early (trials 1-7) and Late (trials 8-14) test trials. Functional MRI percent signal change to the UCS on test trials was used as an index of the amplitude of the neural response to threat. These functional maps were then converted to the Talairach and Tournoux stereotaxic [48] space for group level analyses [49].
Based on prior work [10, 15, 17], we applied an anatomical mask that consisted of the PFC, cingulate cortex, IPL, insula, and amygdala to reduce the number of voxel-wise comparisons for our group level analyses. A repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted to test for a main effect of stimulus (CS+UCS, CS−UCS, and UCS alone) and trial (Early vs. Late), as well as a stimulus x trial interaction. A voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.05 (corrected) was employed by using an uncorrected threshold of p < 0.005 and a cluster volume larger than 510 mm3 (9 voxels of 3.75 x 3.75 x 4.00 mm dimension). Monte Carlo simulations, conducted in AFNI, were used to select these threshold criteria and reject smaller clusters of activation due to chance alone (false positives) [50, 51]. Follow-up t-test comparisons were conducted in SPSS on the mean percent signal change in threat-related brain activation that passed the significance threshold criteria (p < 0.05 Bonferroni corrected) revealed by the ANOVA.
Two different analysis procedures (i.e. correlation and multiple linear regression) were conducted to investigate the relationship between threat-elicited brain activation and behavior (i.e. trait anxiety, UCS expectancy, and unconditioned SCR expression). These analyses were restricted to the brain regions that demonstrated learning-related changes from the ANOVA (i.e. functional regions of interest; ROI). Although these analyses are similar there are important differences (see [17] for additional discussion). In short, separate correlation analyses assessed the relationship between each of our behavioral measures and the mean percent signal change within an ROI as a whole, while the regression analysis evaluated these relationships on a voxel-wise basis. The four participants without measurable SCR data and the two participants without a trait anxiety score were excluded from this regression analysis because there were no data points to include in the model. AlphaSim [47, 51] was used to conduct Monte Carlo simulations limited to the functional ROI revealed by our repeated-measures ANOVA that demonstrated a main effect of stimulus or stimulus x trial interaction. A voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.005 and a cluster volume larger than 225mm3 (4 voxels of 3.75 x 3.75 x 4.00mm dimension) was employed, resulting in a FWE corrected significance threshold of p < 0.05. Prior work has demonstrated a relationship between the magnitude of the fMRI signal response within the amygdala and SCR production during Pavlovian fear conditioning [10, 17, 22, 39-41]. Therefore an anatomical mask was employed to include the amygdala in the group level regression analysis as well.
Prior work suggests that the amygdala and PFC produce and regulate the emotional response to aversive stimuli [19, 38]. Therefore, we completed an additional voxel-wise multiple regression analysis to identify anticipatory PFC, cingulate, and amygdala activity that varied with the threat-related fMRI signal response. This analysis was limited to the functional ROI revealed by our repeated-measures ANOVA. Given that no CS was presented on UCS alone trials to elicit an anticipatory response, this analysis was restricted to CS+UCS and CS−UCS trials. This analysis included regressors for trial type (CS+UCS & CS−UCS), threat-elicited fMRI signal response amplitude, and interaction of trial type and threat-elicited fMRI signal response amplitude. Monte Carlo simulations conducted in AFNI indicated that a voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.005 and a cluster volume larger than 510 mm3 (9 voxels of 3.75 x 3.75 x 4.00 mm dimension) resulted in a FWE corrected significance threshold of p < 0.05. Given our a priori hypotheses and the relatively small volume of the amygdala, we applied a small volume correction using a voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.005 and a cluster volume larger than 112 mm3 (2 voxels of 3.75 x 3.75 x 4.00 mm dimension) for this area (p < 0.05 corrected).
RESULTS
UCS expectancy:
Repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant differences in UCS expectancy during the test trials. The results indicated there was a main effect for stimulus type (F[1,20] = 38.30, p < 0.05) and a main effect for trial (F[1,20] = 33.68, p < 0.05). There was no stimulus by trial interaction (F < 1.00). UCS expectancy was greater during Early test trials on CS+UCS [mean ± SEM (adjusted for between subject variance [52]): 65.85 ± 2.86; t[20] = 5.55, p < 0.05] and CS−UCS (70.60 ± 3.97; t[20] = 5.48, p < 0.05) than on UCS alone trials (34.31 ± 3.89). There was no difference in UCS expectancy for CS+UCS and CS−UCS (t[20] = -1.00) presentations during Early test trials. UCS expectancy was greater during Late test trials on CS+UCS (86.09 ± 2.84; t[20] = 6.20, p < 0.05) and CS−UCS (91.83 ± 2.97; t[20] = 6.29, p < 0.05) compared to UCS alone (48.58 ± 4.56) trials. During the Late test trials, UCS expectancy was also greater during CS−UCS presentations than on CS+UCS presentations (t[20] = -2.31, p < 0.05) (Fig. 2a).
Skin conductance response:
Repeated-measures ANOVA also revealed significant differences in unconditioned SCR expression during the test trials. There was a main effect for stimulus type (F[1,16] = 4.85, p < 0.05). There was also a trend for a main effect of trial (F[1,16] = 4.44, p = 0.051), and a trend for a stimulus by trial interaction (F[1,16] = 4.39, p = 0.052). Follow-up t-test comparisons revealed a diminished threat-elicited SCR for CS+UCS (1.07 ± 0.08; t[16] = -3.05, p < 0.05) and CS−UCS trials (1.38 ± 0.17; t[16] = -2.04, p < 0.05) compared to the UCS alone (1.88 ± 0.22) during Early test trials. There were no significant differences in unconditioned SCR between the CS+UCS and CS−UCS trials (t[16] = -1.49) during Early test trials. Also, there were no differences in unconditioned SCR between the CS+UCS trials (0.98 ± 0.20) and CS−UCS trials (0.90 ± 0.18; t < 1.00) during Late test trials. Unconditioned SCR during CS+UCS (t[16] = -1.08) and CS−UCS (t[16] = -1.38) trials also did not differ from UCS alone trials (1.30 ± 0.15) during the Late test trials (Fig. 2b).
Functional MRI:
Repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant differences in the magnitude of the threat-elicited fMRI signal response within several brain regions (Tables 1 and 2; Fig. 3). Conditioned UCR diminution was observed within the dlPFC, dmPFC, vmPFC, and anterior insula replicating prior work [10, 15, 17] (Table 1; Fig. 3). We also observed potentiation of the UCR within several brain regions including the dlPFC, IPL, and posterior insula (Table 2). Within each of these brain regions the threat-related fMRI signal response demonstrated a main effect for stimulus type (F[20] > 6.06; p < 0.05 corrected). A main effect for trial (i.e. Early vs. Late) was observed within the dlPFC, IPL, and anterior insula (Table 3). A stimulus x trial interaction was observed within the left IPL (Table 2). T-test comparisons were conducted on the mean fMRI signal response from each area of activation that passed the significance threshold (p < 0.05 corrected) for the main effect of stimulus type. There was no difference in the threat-related fMRI signal response for CS+UCS compared to CS−UCS trials within the functional ROIs identified by the ANOVA. The unconditioned fMRI signal response was diminished for the CS+UCS and CS−UCS compared to the UCS alone within left dlPFC, dmPFC, vmPFC, and left anterior insula (Table 1). However, greater activation was observed for CS+UCS and CS−UCS trials compared to UCS alone trials within left dlPFC and bilateral IPL. Additionally, greater activation for CS−UCS vs. UCS alone trials was observed within bilateral posterior insula (Table 2).
Talairach Coordinates | CS+UCS vs. CS–UCS | CS–UCS vs. UCS Alone | CS+UCS vs. UCS Alone | Trait | SCR | UCS Expectancy | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Region | Vol (mm3) | x | y | z | t | t | t | r | r | r |
Dorsolateral PFC | ||||||||||
Left | 3739 | –46.3 | 10.8 | 35.0 | n.s. | –4.23 | –3.81 | 0.04 | –0.04 | –0.17 |
Dorsomedial PFC | 5670 | –1.6 | 24.5 | 45.9 | n.s. | –4.52 | –4.18 | 0.04 | –0.04 | –0.36* |
Ventromedial PFC | 515 | –0.9 | 52.7 | –0.5 | n.s. | –4.33 | –3.96 | –0.02 | 0.05 | –0.50* |
Anterior Insula | ||||||||||
Left | 1046 | –34.6 | 16.3 | 1.7 | n.s. | –3.73 | –3.99 | 0.06 | 0.18 | –0.48* |
Note. Location, volumes, and coordinates from Talairach and Tournoux (1988) for the center of mass for areas of activation. Significance criteria: ANOVA F[20] > 6.06, p < 0.05 (corrected); t[20] p < 0.05 (corrected). Significance criteria for two-tailed correlations:
* indicates p < 0.05 (corrected).
Talairach Coordinates | CS+UCS vs. CS–UCS | CS–UCS vs. UCS Alone | CS+UCS vs. UCS Alone | Trait | SCR | UCS Expectancy | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Region | Vol (mm3) | x | y | z | t | t | t | r | r | r |
Main effect of stimulus type | ||||||||||
Dorsolateral PFC | ||||||||||
Left | 1286 | -7.3 | -14.8 | 51.7 | n.s. | 3.93 | 3.57 | 0.15 | 0.14 | 0.30 |
Inf. Parietal Lobule | ||||||||||
Right | 954 | 56.3 | -29.3 | 28.4 | n.s. | 3.82 | 3.45 | 0.32 | –0.06 | 0.28 |
Left | 2618 | -39.2 | -40.9 | 51.0 | n.s. | 3.85 | 3.44 | 0.15 | –0.07 | 0.43* |
Left | 1054 | -60.9 | -27.0 | 27.7 | n.s. | 4.20 | 3.39 | 0.28 | –0.01 | 0.34 |
Posterior Insula | ||||||||||
Right | 3155 | 41.1 | -7.7 | 10.8 | n.s. | 5.62 | n.s. | 0.16 | –0.02 | 0.28 |
Left | 2106 | -41.9 | -14.2 | 12.6 | n.s. | 4.29 | n.s. | 0.22 | –0.12 | 0.31 |
Stimulus x trial interaction | ||||||||||
Inf. Parietal Lobule | 577 | -42.8 | -34.4 | 38.7 | ||||||
Early test trials | n.s. | n.s. | 4.11 | 0.14 | –0.06 | 0.33 | ||||
Late test trials | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | 0.05 | 0.09 | 0.20 |
Note. Location, volumes, and coordinates from Talairach and Tournoux (1988) for the center of mass for areas of activation. Significance criteria: ANOVA F[20] > 6.06, p < 0.05 (corrected); t[20] p < 0.05 (corrected). Significance criteria for two-tailed correlations:
* indicates p < 0.05 (corrected).
Talairach Coordinates | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Region | Hemisphere | Vol (mm3) | x | y | z |
Dorsolateral PFC | Right | 1072 | 23.7 | 44.0 | 32.0 |
Left | 591 | -45.8 | 9.0 | 29.5 | |
Inf. Parietal Lobule | Right | 553 | 51.0 | -41.9 | 24.7 |
Anterior Insula | Left | 1258 | -44.4 | 21.0 | 0.9 |
Note. Location, volumes, and coordinates from Talairach and Tournoux (1988) for the center of mass for areas of activation. Significance criteria: F[20] > 10.00; p < 0.05 (corrected).
A correlation analysis was performed on the mean percent signal change from the functional ROIs revealed by the ANOVA and our behavioral measures (i.e. trait anxiety level, UCS expectancy, and unconditioned SCR expression) (Tables 1 and 2; p < 0.05 Bonferroni corrected). The threat-related percent signal change within the functional ROI did not vary with trait anxiety or unconditioned SCR. These findings replicate prior work that used CS presentations that were easy to discriminate during Pavlovian fear conditioning [17]. However, a significant correlation was observed between UCS expectancy and the threat-elicited fMRI signal response within the dmPFC, vmPFC, left IPL, and left anterior insula (Tables 1 and 2), generally consistent with prior research [17]. A voxel-wise multiple linear regression analysis was conducted to evaluate brain activity within the functional ROI that varied with individual differences in behavior. The amygdala was also included in this analysis because prior work suggests this brain region mediates learning-related changes in SCR production [22, 39–41]. The regression model accounted for stimulus type, trait anxiety, UCS expectancy, and unconditioned SCR. There were no brain regions within the functional ROI that varied with behavior (i.e. trait anxiety, UCS expectancy, or unconditioned SCR amplitude).
We also conducted a group level regression analysis to investigate whether anticipatory activation (i.e. the CR) within the PFC, cingulate, and amygdala varied with threat-related activity (i.e. the UCR) from brain regions that demonstrated conditioned UCR diminution (Table 1). Anticipatory activity within dlPFC, dmPFC, vmPFC, ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), posterior cingulate, and amygdala revealed a negative relationship with the threat-elicited fMRI signal response (i.e. UCR) within many of the brain regions in which conditioned UCR diminution was observed (Table 4). Anticipatory activation within left vlPFC was negatively correlated with the threat response within each of the brain areas that showed UCR diminution. This effect was also observed between anticipatory activation within the right vlPFC and threat-related activity within each of the functional ROI that showed UCR diminution, except for the left dlPFC (Table 4). A negative relationship was also observed between anticipatory activity within vmPFC and the threat-related response within left dlPFC, dmPFC, and left anterior insula (Fig. 4a and c). A similar pattern was observed between anticipatory activation within dlPFC, dmPFC, vlPFC, and PCC and the threat-related response within vmPFC (Fig. 4e). Finally, anticipatory amygdala activity showed a similar negative relationship with the threat-related response within dmPFC and left anterior insula (Fig. 4b and d).
DISCUSSION
The ability to learn cues that predict threats is critical to survival. These cue-threat relationships are established when outcomes violate our expectations [53]. These relationships are supported by associative learning and expectancy-related processes that may somewhat independently influence the magnitude of the threat-elicited response [10-12, 15, 17]. Therefore, the present study used CS presentations that were difficult to discriminate to investigate the neural processes that support conditioned UCR diminution in the absence of differential UCS expectancies to better understand associative learning processes that mediate UCR diminution.
In the present study, we observed learning-related changes in UCS expectancy and unconditioned SCR expression. By design, UCS expectancy was high on CS+UCS and CS−UCS trials. In contrast, UCS expectancy on UCS alone trials was rated around 50 (Fig. 2a). These findings demonstrate that volunteers expected the UCS following the CS+ and CS−, but remained uncertain about the timing of the UCS alone during the conditioning session. UCS expectancy was also associated with the amplitude of threat-elicited SCR expression. Specifically, unconditioned SCR amplitude was decreased on CS+UCS and CS−UCS trials (when UCS expectancy was high) compared to the UCS alone (when UCS expectancy was lower) on Early test trials (Fig. 2b). These findings are consistent with prior work that has shown a decreased UCR when the UCS is predictable vs. unpredictable [8, 9, 11, 54]. Based on our pilot data, we also expected to observe greater unconditioned SCR diminution during CS+UCS than CS−UCS trials in this study. Findings of differential SCR, with equivalent UCS expectancy, would allow us to address questions related to learning independent of conscious expectations. However, no difference in unconditioned SCR expression was observed between CS+UCS and CS−UCS trials. Therefore, there is no evidence of conditioned UCR diminution that is independent of UCS expectancy in the present study. The lack of differential SCRs on CS+UCS vs. CS−UCS trials is likely due to the lack of discriminative control gained by the CS. Given that the CS+ and CS− were difficult to discriminate, evidenced by high expectancy ratings, participants appear to have interpreted the acquisition phase as a 50% reinforcement schedule of a single CS rather than separate presentations of a CS+ and CS−. Therefore, the lack of differential unconditioned SCRs during CS+UCS compared to CS−UCS trials appears to be due to a deficit in learning the CS discrimination.
Although there is no evidence of learning, independent of expectancy, we did observe conditioned UCR diminution in other contrasts in the present study. Unconditioned SCR amplitude was diminished on CS+UCS and CS−UCS trials compared to the UCS alone trials. This pattern was also observed in the fMRI signal within the left dlPFC (z = 35.0), dmPFC, vmPFC, and left anterior insula. These findings are generally consistent with prior studies that have shown UCR diminution within these brain regions [10, 15, 17]. However, contrary to our prior work, an enhanced threat-related fMRI response was observed within other brain regions including a more superior region of left dlPFC (z = 51.7), bilateral IPL, and bilateral posterior insula. Similar to the SCR data, there were no differences between CS+UCS and CS−UCS trials within any of the brain regions that demonstrated learning-related changes in the threat-elicited fMRI signal response (Tables 1 and 2). Taken together, the present findings replicate prior studies that have shown learning-related changes in brain activation that resemble the pattern of the emotional response produced [15, 17, 22, 39, 40].
Prior work suggests that UCS expectancy modulates the magnitude of the threat-related fMRI signal response [10, 15, 17]. Prior work has also shown that conscious expectation influences the emotional response produced by a threat [10, 11, 15–17]. Similar results were observed in the current study. Consistent with our prior work, a negative relationship was observed between UCS expectancy and brain activity in regions that showed conditioned UCR diminution [17]. Specifically, a negative relationship was observed between UCS expectancy and brain activation within dmPFC, vmPFC, and left anterior insula (Table 1). However, a positive relationship was observed between UCS expectancy and the amplitude of the threat-related fMRI signal response within left IPL (Table 2). This relationship between the unconditioned fMRI signal response and UCS expectancy was not observed in our group level multiple linear regression analysis. The regression analysis was conducted on a voxel-wise basis and accounted for additional measures of interest (e.g. stimulus type). These findings suggest that UCS expectancy varied with the mean percent signal change of the threat-related fMRI signal response within the ROIs as a whole. However, UCS expectancy did not explain unique variance within the threat-related fMRI signal response within these brain regions. These findings replicate prior conditioning work that employed CS presentations that were easy to discriminate [17]. In addition, these data support previous research that suggests that regions of the PFC support associative learning processes [55, 56].
The PFC appears to support top-down processes that are important for emotion regulation. Prior work has demonstrated a negative relationship between anticipatory PFC activity and the threat-related response within the amygdala [17, 38]. Specifically, as anticipatory PFC activity increased, the neural response to the threat decreased [17]. The data from the current study are generally consistent with this previous research. In the current study, a negative relationship was observed between anticipatory activation within the PFC and threat-related activity within brain regions that showed UCR diminution (Table 1). Specifically, as anticipatory activity within dlPFC, dmPFC, vlPFC, vmPFC, and the amygdala increased the threat-related response within left dlPFC, dmPFC, vmPFC, and anterior insula decreased (Table 4, Fig. 4). These findings demonstrate that anticipatory brain activity affects the response elicited by a threat. The primary focus of fear conditioning research has typically been centered on the anticipatory response to a threat. This prior work has contributed to the development of cognitive, behavioral, and drug interventions [28, 57-59] and has provided a better understanding of the aberrant top-down emotion regulation processes that are observed in conditions such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [27, 29, 60-62]. The current findings demonstrate neural mechanisms that influence the threat-related emotional response and may offer novel insights for future clinical research on emotion regulation.
In summary, conditioned diminution of the unconditioned SCR and fMRI signal response was observed during Pavlovian fear conditioning. UCR diminution was observed within left dlPFC, dmPFC, vmPFC, and left anterior insula. Consistent with prior work, many of the brain regions that showed learning-related changes in the threat-related fMRI signal response varied with UCS expectancy [10, 15, 17]. Further, a negative relationship was observed between the brain regions that showed UCR diminution and anticipatory PFC activation (i.e. dlPFC, dmPFC, vlPFC, and vmPFC). This finding supports prior work that suggests top-down mechanisms provided by the PFC inhibit the emotional response produced by a threat. The findings from the current study provide a better understanding of the neural mechanisms that support associative learning processes that modulate the response to a threat.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was supported by the University of Alabama at Birmingham Faculty Development Grant Program and NIH R01 MH098348 (DCK).